Abstract
Although there is evidence that adopted persons fare worse than non-adoptees in terms of relational adjustment and
other psychosocial variables, some studies have produced null results, and others have demonstrated the wide variability in
adjustment among adoptees.
Several authors in this area have discussed issues pertaining to loss, abandonment, and rejection. However, researchers have not systematically examined the impact of adoption on adults' attachment security
and relationship outcomes, or the moderating role of family and search/reunion experiences.
This paper reports on the first phase of a longitudinal study of adults who were adopted as infants, and a comparison
sample of adults who were raised by both biological parents. Measures of attachment
styles and attachment dimensions indicated less security in the adopted sample than the comparison sample. However, variability was somewhat greater among adoptees, and those who had not searched for birth relatives
were generally similar to the comparison sample. Within the adopted sample, attachment
security was related to perceptions of childhood experiences and current relationships with adoptive parents and, to a lesser
extent, relationships with birth mothers. Ongoing analyses will focus on stability
and change in relationship variables, and in-depth exploration of adopted persons' experiences.
Presentation
Although there is evidence that adopted persons fare worse than non-adoptees in terms of relational adjustment and
other psychosocial variables, some studies have produced null results, and others have demonstrated the wide variability in
adjustment among adoptees. Several authors in this area have discussed issues
pertaining to loss, abandonment, and rejection. However, researchers have not
systematically examined the impact of adoption on adults' attachment security and relationship outcomes, or the moderating
role of family and search/reunion experiences. This paper reports on the first
phase of a longitudinal study of adults who were adopted as infants, and a comparison sample of adults who were raised by
both biological parents. Measures of attachment styles and attachment dimensions
indicated less security in the adopted sample than the comparison sample. However,
variability was somewhat greater among adoptees, and those who had not searched for birth relatives were generally similar
to the comparison sample. Within the adopted sample, attachment security was
related to perceptions of childhood experiences and current relationships with adoptive parents and, to a lesser extent, relationships
with birth mothers. Ongoing analyses will focus on stability and change in relationship
variables, and in-depth exploration of adopted persons' experiences.
Both theory and research
highlight the importance of personal relationships in meeting needs for comfort and security, and in promoting well being
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Weiss, 1991). Although issues regarding the quality
and stability of relationships are of general importance, there are compelling reasons for studying the relationship experiences
and concerns of adult adoptees. Adoptees have lost the major person(s) with whom
attachments normally form (i.e., biological parents). Further, with recent changes
in legislation, more adoptees are now searching for birth relatives, and this process may entail further loss and rejection. Hence, adoption may be a risk factor for relationship difficulties in adult life.
Adoption, Family Experiences and Adjustment
Many researchers
have investigated the extent to which adoption is a risk factor for general adjustment difficulties. There is evidence that adoptees are over-represented in clinical populations, and report higher levels
of psychosocial difficulties (e.g., self-esteem, depression) than non-adoptees (Borders, Penny, & Portnoy, 2000; Cubito
& Obremski-Brandon, 2000; Levy-Shiff, 2001; Wierzbicki, 1993). However, Collishaw,
Maughan, and Pickles (1998) reported that adoptees did not differ from the general population in terms of psychological distress,
and Borders et al. (2000) found no difference between adoptees and their friends in terms of life satisfaction. Further, Sharma, McGue, and Benson (1996) reported higher levels of prosocial behaviour among adopted than
nonadopted adolescents. Although methodological differences may explain some
of these mixed results, another explanation is that the association between adoption status and psychosocial outcomes is moderated
by search status and family experiences. On average, adoptees who have searched
for birth parents (‘searchers’) have lower self-esteem than non-searchers (Aumend & Barrett, 1984; Borders
et al., 2000; Sobol & Cardiff, 1983). Further, Sobol and Cardiff (1983) found a trend among non-searchers such that those who had less favourable relationships with adoptive parents
were more likely to report a desire to search in the future. These findings suggest
that a well-functioning adoptive family acts as a buffer against psychosocial difficulties.
Indeed, data suggest that when the adoptive family is expressive and supportive, adoptees are more likely to develop
healthy self-esteem (Kelly, Towner-Thyrum, Rigby, & Martin, 1998; Levy-Shiff, 2001).
If adoption is a risk factor for psychosocial difficulties, at least for some adoptees, then many adoptees may
also experience difficulties in interpersonal relationships. Issues concerning
loss and betrayal (which are inherently relational) are central to the adoption experience.
Not only have adoptees lost their birth parents; they have also lost other birth relatives, knowledge of their genetic
heritage, and a sense of being biologically tied to significant others (Jones, 1997; Schechter & Bertocci, 1990). In cases where reunion is unsuccessful, adoptees may feel rejected again, and thus
experience a double loss. It is widely accepted that parental loss through death
or divorce predicts is linked to insecurity and interpersonal difficulties (Kobak, 1999), but the losses associated with adoption
have unique features that may predispose individuals to relationship problems. Specifically,
these losses are covert and often unacknowledged or underestimated, and may entail a sense of betrayal, abandonment and mistrust
(Brodzinsky, 1990; Jones, 1997; Nickman, 1985). Although several studies have
examined the family relationships of adoptees, few have assessed the impact of adoption on the peer relationships of adult
adoptees. As argued next, attachment theory is particularly well suited to addressing
these issues.
The Adult Attachment Perspective
Some years
ago, Hazan and Shaver (1987) argued that adults’ close relationships (especially those with intimate partners) share
important emotional, behavioural and functional similarities with the bonds that form between infants and their caregivers. They also argued that the concept of ‘attachment style’ (secure, avoidant,
anxious/ambivalent) is relevant to both types of relationships. That is, individual
differences in adult security predict key relationship processes and reflect, in part, childhood experiences with attachment
figures. Subsequent research has generally supported these propositions (Feeney,
1999). Further, measures of adult attachment have evolved rapidly: Although some
studies still rely on categorical measures (e.g., secure, preoccupied, fearful and dismissing), there has been a move toward
the use of more reliable multiple-item measures.
Recently, Edens and Cavell (1999) made a strong case for the utility of attachment theory in the study
of adoption. They argued that current conceptualisations of adult attachment
are directly relevant to relationship phenomena unique to adoptees, including loss of biological ties, and the potential for
search and reunion. To date, however, Borders et al. (2000) are the only researchers
to have systematically investigated attachment security in adult adoptees. These
researchers studied adoptees and their non-adopted friends. Although the two
groups were similar in terms of marital satisfaction and sensitivity to rejection, they differed with regard to adult attachment
and social support. Adoptees (regardless of search status) were over-represented
in the preoccupied and fearful attachment groups, and under-represented in the secure group.
Adoptees also reported less social support than their non-adopted friends, although this association was moderated
by search status: Searchers reported less support than non-searchers and non-adopted respondents. This study provides an important first step in exploring the link between adoption and adult attachment,
but was limited by its cross-sectional nature, its reliance on a categorical measure of attachment, and its failure to fully
consider the role of early parenting and ongoing relationship experiences.
In summary, despite the
considerable literature on issues of infant attachment, loss, rejection, abandonment and trust in adoptees, no study has comprehensively
explored the impact of adoption on dimensions of attachment security and relationship outcomes in adulthood, or the moderating
role of family and search/reunion experiences. Such studies are essential in
order to develop best practice for adult adoptees who may be at risk of relationship problems.
This paper reports on the first phase of a study addressing these issues. We
expected that adults who were adopted as infants would report higher levels of attachment insecurity than adults who grew
up with both biological parents (Hypothesis 1a). However, the adopted sample
was also expected to show greater variability on attachment measures (Hypothesis 1b).
Within the adopted sample, insecurity was expected to be higher for those who had searched for birth relatives (Hypothesis
2), and for those who perceived relationships with adoptive parents and birth mothers in a more negative light (Hypothesis
3).
Method
Participants
Participants were
140 adults who were adopted as infants, and a comparison sample of 128 adults who grew up with both biological parents. Adopted participants were required to have been adopted in Australia, by strangers, and to have lived with both adoptive
parents for the first 16 years of life. The demographic characteristics of the
two samples are shown in Table 1. Analysis of variance indicated that the samples
were similar in terms of age, and frequency comparisons revealed similar patterns of gender, relationship status, parental
status, education level, and employment status.
Measures
As part of a larger
study, all participants completed measures of attachment security. In addition,
adoptees reported on relationships in the adoptive family (parental bonding, discussion of the adoption, current emotional
closeness), and on their search and reunion experiences.
Attachment security
Current attachment
security was measured in two ways. First, attachment style was assessed by asking
participants to endorse one of the four attachment descriptions (secure, preoccupied, dismissing, fearful), developed by Bartholomew
and Horowitz (1991). Second, participants completed the 40-item Attachment Style
Questionnaire (ASQ), developed by Feeney, Noller, and Hanrahan (1994).
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of
comparison and adopted samples
Variable |
Comparison |
Adopted |
age |
M = 37.80 years |
M = 37.76 years |
gender |
75.00% females |
77.88% females |
relationship
status |
32.50% single
10.83% de facto
45.00% married
11.67% sep/ divorced |
25.41% single
12.30% de facto
44.26% married
18.03% sep/ divorced |
parental status |
48.82% with children |
57.14% with children |
education level |
9.38% some high school
6.25% Year 12
24.22% some further study
60.16% university degree |
9.29% some high school
9.29% Year 12
35.71% some further study
45.71% university degree |
employment
status |
43.31% full-time
32.28% part-time
24.41% not employed |
50.00% full-time
35.00% part-time
15.00% not employed |
The ASQ measures five dimensions of adult attachment: confidence in self and others (8 items; e.g., ‘I
feel confident about relating to others’), discomfort with closeness (10 items; e.g., ‘I prefer to keep to myself’),
need for approval (7 items; e.g., ‘It’s important to me that others like me’), preoccupation with relationships
(8 items; e.g., ‘I worry a lot about my relationships’), and relationships as secondary to achievement (7 items;
e.g., ‘Achieving things is more important than building relationships’).
Each item is rated on a 6-point scale, from 1 (totally disagree) to 6 (totally agree). All
five scales were reliable, with alpha coefficients ranging from .74 to .88.
Parental bonding
The Parental Bonding
Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979) contains 25 items describing various parental attitudes and behaviours. This measure requires participants to think back over the first 16 years of life,
and to rate each item (separately for mother and father) from 0 (very unlike this parent)
to 3 (very like this parent). Adoptees
answered these questions with respect to their adoptive parents. The PBI yields
scores on the dimensions of care (12 items, e.g., ‘spoke to me with a warm and friendly voice’), and overprotection
(13 items, e.g., ‘tried to control everything I did’). Both scales
were highly reliable, with alpha coefficients exceeding .90 for each parent.
Additional questions about the adoptive family.
In addition, adoptees
were asked about the adoptive family’s attitude toward discussing the adoption (1 = the
topic was taboo or the source of lies and misinformation) to 3 = discussion was
open and honest). They also rated current emotional closeness to the adoptive
mother and adoptive father (1 = extremely distant to 6 = extremely close).
Search and reunion experiences
Adoptees answered a
series of questions about their search and reunion experiences, five of which are considered in this paper. First, those who had searched for birth relatives rated how supportive the adoptive mother and adoptive
father were of their decision to search (1 = extremely opposed to 6 = very supportive). Finally, those who had met their birth mothers
rated their satisfaction with the initial reunion and satisfaction with the current relationship (1 = extremely dissatisfying to 6 = extremely satisfying), and emotional
closeness of the current relationship (1 = extremely distant to 6 = extremely close).
Procedure
For both samples,
participants were recruited through the first-year Psychology pools at the University of Queensland and University of Southern Queensland, through brochures and flyers placed on university campuses and in various community centres, and through advertisements
in local media. Those interested in participating were informed of the purpose
and confidential nature of the study, and were mailed a questionnaire package with a pre-paid envelope for returning the materials. The major sections of the questionnaire (attachment security, relationships with adoptive
parents, search and reunion experiences) were presented in counterbalanced order.
Results
Adoption and Attachment Characteristics
The association between
adoption and attachment security was assessed in two ways. First, a frequency
comparison was conducted, relating sample (comparison versus adopted) to the four-group (forced-choice) measure of attachment
style. The association between sample and attachment category was significant,
2 (3) = 19.71, p
< .001. Adopted persons represented only 38% of the secure group; in contrast,
they represented 59% of the dismissing group, 64% of the preoccupied group, and 72% of the fearful group.
Second, MANOVA was used
to assess differences between the samples on the five scales of the ASQ. This
analysis revealed a significant overall difference, multivariate F (5, 262) = 4.10,
p < .001; further, univariate tests showed that this difference applied to all
five scales. Adopted persons obtained lower scores than comparison persons on
confidence, and higher scores on all remaining attachment scales (see Table 2, top rows).
The multivariate test of homogeneity variance was marginally significant, indicating greater variability in attachment
scores within the adopted sample than the comparison sample. However, this effect
applied only to the confidence scale.
Search Status
To assess the role of
search status, a more fine-grained MANOVA was conducted in which searchers (n =
106), nonsearchers (n = 33) and comparison participants (n = 126) were compared on the attachment scales. Significant differences
emerged on all scales except for relationships as secondary. Post hoc (Tukey)
tests showed a consistent pattern, involving significant differences between searchers and comparison participants (see Table
2): Searchers reported lower levels of confidence, and higher levels of discomfort, preoccupation, and need for approval. Interestingly, non-searchers and comparison participants did not differ on any of
the five scales.
Table 2
Mean scores and standard deviations
on attachment scales according to group
|
Conf. |
Disc. |
Relate
second. |
Need
approv. |
Preocc. |
Comparison |
4.64
(0.80) |
3.20
(0.91) |
2.08
(0.75) |
2.95
(0.91) |
3.14
(0.94) |
Adopted |
4.19
(1.02) |
3.51
(1.02) |
2.26
(0.81) |
3.35
(1.03) |
3.56
(0.97) |
Searchers |
4.17
(0.98) |
3.53
(1.05) |
2.24
(0.84) |
3.41
(1.05) |
3.64
(0.93) |
Non-searchers |
4.34
(1.12) |
3.44
(0.93) |
2.34
(0.72) |
3.17
(1.00) |
3.31
(1.07) |
Note. Conf. = Confidence,
Disc. = Discomfort, Relate second. = Relationship as secondary, Need approv. = Need for approval, Preocc. = Preoccupation
Relationships with Adoptive Parents
To assess the role of
relationships with adoptive parents, the attachment scales were correlated with reports of childhood relationships with adoptive
parents, openness of discussion, and current emotional closeness (Table 3). All
attachment scales were associated with reports of relationships with adoptive parents, although the results were strongest
for confidence and discomfort. In terms of parental bonding, confidence was related
positively to parental care (from both adoptive mother and adoptive father), and negatively to parental overprotection. Further, confidence was related positively to open discussion of adoption, and to
ratings of current emotional closeness. Conversely, discomfort was related negatively
to parental care, open discussion and emotional closeness, and positively to parental overprotection.
Table 3
Correlations between attachment security and reports of adoptive parents
Relationship variable |
Conf. |
Disc. |
Relate
second. |
Need
approv. |
Preocc. |
Parental bonding |
|
|
|
|
|
Maternal care |
.43*** |
-.40*** |
-.16+ |
-.24** |
-.26** |
Paternal care |
.31*** |
-.25** |
-.06 |
-.30*** |
-.24** |
Maternal overprot. |
-.40*** |
.37*** |
.14 |
.27*** |
.25** |
Paternal overprot. |
-.28*** |
.25** |
-.01 |
.26** |
.10 |
Open discussion |
.28** |
-.25** |
-.18* |
-.12 |
-.16 |
Closeness to mother |
.41*** |
-.43*** |
-.20* |
-.16 |
-.22* |
Closeness to father |
.34*** |
-.32*** |
-.15 |
-.10 |
-.19 |
Note. Conf. = Confidence,
Disc. = Discomfort, Relate second. = Relationship as secondary, Need approv. = Need for approval, Preocc. = Preoccupation;
+ p < .06, * p
< .05, ** p < .01, *** p <
.001
Search and reunion experiences
Finally, the five attachment
scales were correlated with reports of adoptive parents’ support for the search (n
= 106), and, for those who had met their birth mother (n = 75), with ratings of
that relationship. Associations with these items were more scattered (Table 4). However, confidence was associated with parental support for the search (both maternal
and parental), and with perceptions of a more satisfying reunion. Conversely,
discomfort was associated with lack of maternal support for the search, and with perceptions of a less satisfying reunion
Table 4
Correlations between attachment security and adoptees’ reports of search and reunion
Relationship variable |
Conf. |
Disc. |
Relate
second. |
Need
approv. |
Preocc. |
Adoptive parents |
|
|
|
|
|
Mother’s support
for search |
.38** |
-.31** |
-.21 |
-.09 |
-.20 |
Father’s support
for search |
.28* |
-.20 |
-.02 |
-.05 |
.01 |
Birth mother |
|
|
|
|
|
satisfaction (reunion) |
.26* |
-.28* |
-.18 |
-.11 |
-.11 |
satisfaction (current) |
.14 |
-.11 |
-.11 |
-.10 |
-.01 |
closeness |
.14 |
-.12 |
-.11 |
.02 |
.03 |
Note. Conf. = Confidence,
Disc. = Discomfort, Relate second. = Relationship as secondary, Need approv. = Need for approval, Preocc. = Preoccupation;
Discussion
Before discussing the
results in more detail, it is important to acknowledge the limitations of the study.
Sample size was relatively small for some categories (e.g., those who had met their birth mothers). Further, because these data are cross-sectional in nature, being drawn from Phase 1 of the study. This issue complicates, in particular, interpretations of the associations between
measures of attachment security and relationships with birth mothers. It is possible, for example, that avoidant attachment
(which is reflected in high levels of discomfort with closeness) colours reports of the reunion experience, rather than the
reverse.
Overall, the results
support the suggestion that insecure attachment is more widespread among adoptees than among the general population. Adoptees
scored lower than comparison participants on confidence in self and others, and higher on all five scales tapping dimensions
of insecurity. Adoptees were also over-represented in the insecure attachment
categories. This over-representation applied particularly to the fearful style. Attachment
theory suggests that fearful attachment represents the most negative pattern of working models (Bartholomew & Horowitz,
1991), and empirical research on the characteristics of the four styles supports this assertion (e.g., Feeney, Noller, &
Hanrahan, 1994). These findings on the attachment characteristics of the samples
support Hypothesis 1a, and fit with the suggestion that attachment theory provides a useful perspective on the relationship
issues that arise for adoptees, including loss of biological ties and the potential for search and reunion (Edens & Cavell,
1999).
However, consistent
with Hypothesis 1b, the results also pointed to the wide variability in adoptees’ responses to measures of attachment
security. Further, on four of the five attachment scales, it was only those who
had searched for birth relatives who reported higher levels of insecurity than the comparison sample. These results fit with previous studies linking search status to indices of psychological adjustment (e.g.,
Sobol & Cardiff, 1983), and support Hypothesis 2.
As expected (Hypothesis
3), our results supported the association between adult attachment security and positive relationships with adoptive parents
(high care, low overprotection, open discussion of adoption, and current emotional closeness).
These findings fit with the broader literature, which has established the link between responsive parenting and offspring’s
attachment security (e.g., Rothbard & Shaver, 1994).
To a lesser extent,
the results also point to the relevance of relationships with birth mothers, at least in terms of the initial reunion experience. The weaker nature of the findings pertaining to birth mothers probably reflects not
only the smaller sample size, but also the less formative nature of these relationships, compared to those that are established
with primary caregivers and involve regular and sustained interaction.
Finally, it is worth
noting some of the future directions in this research project. We are interested
in the stability of adult attachment over time, given previous suggestions that working models may be more tentative and unstable
in insecure individuals. Further, we have collected a large body of qualitative
data tapping relationships with adoptive parents and experiences of search and reunion, and these data will be supplemented
by in-depth interviews with the adoptees. One of the complex issues to be addressed
in the interviews concerns adoptees’ experiences of negotiating roles, relationships, and
boundaries between birth and adoptive families. These issues need to be
addressed in order to develop best practice guidelines for those who work with adult adoptees.
References
Aumend, S. A., & Barrett, M. C. (1984). Self-concept and attitudes toward adoption: A comparison of searching and nonsearching adult adoptees. Child Welfare, 63, 251-259.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category
model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments
as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
Borders, L. D., Penny, J. M., & Portnoy, F. (2000). Adult adoptees and their friends: Current functioning and psychosocial well-being. Family Relations, 49, 407-418.
Brodzinsky, D. M. (1990).
A stress and coping model of adoption adjustment. In D. M. Brodzinsky
& M. D. Schechter (Eds.), The psychology of adoption (pp. 3-24). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., & Pickles, A. (1998). Infant adoption: Psychosocial outcomes in adulthood. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 57-65.
Cubito, D. S., & Obremski-Brandon, K. (2000). Psychological adjustment in adult adoptees: Assessment of distress, depression, and anger. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 70, 408-413.
Edens, J. F., & Cavell, T. A. (1999). A review and reformulation of adoptive relationships from an attachment perspective. Adoption Quarterly, 3, 43-70.
Feeney, J. A. (1999).
Adult romantic attachment and couple relationships. In J. Cassidy &
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The handbook of attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 355-377). New York: Guilford.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Hanrahan, M. (1994). Assessing
adult attachment: Developments in the conceptualization of security and insecurity.
In M. B. Sperling & W. H. Berman (Eds.), Attachment in adults: Theory, assessment,
and treatment (pp.128-152). New
York: Guilford.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized
as an attachment process. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.
Jones, A. (1997). Issues
relevant to therapy with adoptees. Psychotherapy,
34, 64-68.
Kelly, M. M., Towner-Thyrum, E., Rigby, A., & Martin, B. (1998). Adjustment and identity formation in adopted and nonadopted young adults: Contributions
of family environment. American Journal
of Orthopsychiatry, 68, 497-500.
Kobak, R. (1999).
The emotional dynamics of disruptions in attchment relationships: Implications for theory, research, and clinical intervention.
In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
and clinical applications (pp. 355-377). New York: Guilford.
Levy-Shiff, R. (2001). Psychological
adjustment of adoptees in adulthood: Family environment and adoption-related correlates.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 97-104.
Nickman, S. L. (1985). Losses
in adoption: The need for dialogue. Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child, 40, 365-398.
Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A Parental Bonding Instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1-10.
Rothbard, J. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Continuity
of attachment across the life span. In M. B. Sperling & W. H. Berman (Eds.),
Attachment in adults: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 31-71). New York: Guilford.
Schechter, M. D., & Bertocci, D. (1990). The meaning of the search. In D. M. Brodzinsky & M. D.
Schechter (Eds.), The psychology of adoption (pp. 62-90). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sharma, A. R., McGue, M. K., & Benson, P. L. (1996). The emotional and behavioral adjustment of United States adopted adolescents: Part 1. An overview. Children
and Youth Services Review, 18, 83-100.
Sobol, M. P., & Cardiff, J. (1983). A sociopsychological
investigation of adult adoptees’ search for birth parents. Family Relations, 32, 477-483.
Weiss, R. S. (1991). The attachment bond in childhood and adulthood. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde,
& P. Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 66-76). London: Tavistock/Routledge.
Wierzbicki, M. (1993). Psychological
adjustment of adoptees: A meta-analysis. Journal
of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 447-454.